SEP 26, 2017 04:41 AM PDT

How Language Describes Color Differences

While the eye can take in hundreds of thousands of different shades and colors and the brain can process them, noting even small differences in shades, the way language is used to describe color varies depending on what native language is spoken.

In some dialects there can be as few as three or four words to describe shades and colors, but in other cultures there are several different categories, with specific words for all the different hues. In a new study from MIT, cognitive scientists looked at how different speakers described colors on the spectrum and found a pattern across 100 different languages.

In language, colors along the spectrum are often described differently depending on where on the range they fall. Warmer colors, like orange, red and yellow a new study, MIT cognitive scientists have found that languages tend to divide the “warm” part of the color spectrum into more color words, such as orange, yellow, and red, are described with many more words and categories, than the blues and greens that are on the “cooler” parts of the spectrum. First author of the study, Edward Gibson, a professor of brain and cognitive sciences at MIT explained his research, stating, “When we look at it, it turns out it’s the same across every language that we studied. Every language has this amazing similar ordering of colors, so that reds are more consistently communicated than greens or blues. What this means is that human languages divide that space in a skewed way. In all languages, people preferentially bring color words into the warmer parts of the space and they don’t bring them into the cooler colors.”

A possible explanation for this could be that warmer colors tend to be more obvious, such as a red rose that springs out of a bunch of green stems. The red color is more noticeable, whereas blues and greens tend to be background colors. Gibson first became aware of the differences in how colors are defined with more or less words and descriptors when he came across a study of the Tsimane’ people who belong to a tribe that lives in the thick remote jungle of Bolivia. In this tribe, there are common and consistent words in the Tsimane’ language for colors like red, black or white, but among members of the tribe, describing and naming other shades on the spectrum like greens or blues is less standardized.

When the team at MIT looked at other languages, they found much the same trend. They took color chips with 80 shades along the spectrum and asked the Tsimane’ people to name or describe the colors. When that data had been collected, they developed what is called an “information technique” to quantify how consistently a color was described both across different languages and within the same language group. This factor was called “surprisal.” The more consistently a shade was described, the lower the surprisal factor. Blues and greens were high on the surprisal scale since across Spanish, English and Tsimane’ there was very little consistency in these cooler colors. In contrast, the exact opposite was found with warmer colors, with a high level of consistent descriptors of reds and yellows. This pattern continued across 100 other languages as well, based on data the team pulled from the World Color Survey.

The research suggests that these consistency factors might be due to the fast that warmer colors are perceived by the brain as being more obvious and are found more in the foreground of pictures and examples, where the cooler blues and greens tend to be in the background and not as noticeable. Gibson hopes to expand the research into different climates, to see if those environments differ with what’s in the background and what is perceived and described with more variety. Understanding how the brain processes color, especially among different cultures and in different languages could add some scientific weight to a concept that is sometimes seen as very subjective. If patterns across cultures and language develop, then it can be inferred that color perception is a function of the brain and not just a preference or opinion

The video below has more information, check it out.

Sources: MIT, The Boston Globe, The World Color Survey, PNAS 

About the Author
  • I'm a writer living in the Boston area. My interests include cancer research, cardiology and neuroscience. I want to be part of using the Internet and social media to educate professionals and patients in a collaborative environment.
You May Also Like
OCT 04, 2018
Neuroscience
OCT 04, 2018
Breast Milk is Brain Food for Preemies
A due date for a baby represents great hope to parents and families. Any due date is an educated guess, and some babies arrive a little early or a little l...
OCT 10, 2018
Videos
OCT 10, 2018
Are Personality Tests Scientifically Accurate?
Personality tests like the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory and others are used to find out what a person is like, internally. They measure reac...
OCT 11, 2018
Neuroscience
OCT 11, 2018
Can Sleep Deprivation in Teens Increase Risk-Taking?
Getting enough sleep is a healthy habit for anyone, but in adolescents it's crucial. Brain development is happening at a rapid pace during the teen yea...
NOV 12, 2018
Neuroscience
NOV 12, 2018
Role Of Choroid Plexus Protein Klotho In Aging
Inflammaging, describes the increase in an inflammatory alteration in the brain, due to aging and predicts the morbidity and mortality in older humans....
NOV 18, 2018
Neuroscience
NOV 18, 2018
How does the brain know when we are full?
Feeling full or satiation is conveyed to the brain by the gut hormones via the enteric neuronal afferents and the endocrine feedback pathways....
NOV 19, 2018
Plants & Animals
NOV 19, 2018
Communal Rearing Better Prepares Mice for the Real World, Study Finds
Researchers have long understood early-life experiences to influence actions and behavior later in life for humans, but can be same be said about animals?...
Loading Comments...