JAN 06, 2014 12:00 AM PST

Are Brain-Training Programs Effective?

WRITTEN BY: Jen Ellis
"Brain Training" programs and apps have been gaining in popularity in recent years. For those of you who thought that's what high school and college were for, this variety of brain training refers to the use of relatively simple mind exercises-games and tasks for computers and mobile devices that serve as quick mental challenges to improve cognitive ability.

The games are designed to address a specific category of brain function such as attention, working memory, or problem solving capabilities. Progress in a particular field is measured and tracked over time based on performance changes in these tasks (typically by finishing tasks quicker, or finishing more of them in a set time.)

There's little doubt that these measurements can track tangible improvements in that particular task-but do they translate to general improvements in brain function? In other words, does your increasing ability to recall random phrases in a game make it more likely you'll get home from the grocery store without forgetting the milk and eggs, or that you'll remember the names of everyone you met at your sister's wedding?

Groups like Lumosity, one of the most well-known sites offering brain-training exercises, can point to a significant amount of scientific research to back up their claims. However, at least in one area, a recent study by researchers at Oregon University suggests that transferring game improvements to general cognitive improvement can't necessarily be generalized. These findings were published in the Journal of Neuroscience.

The team studied inhibitory control through a task known as a "stop signal task." Participants from 18-30 years in age were instructed to push an arrow key as fast as possible upon receiving a "go" signal. During the trials, one-quarter of the participants received a "stop" signal instructing them to stop pressing the arrow key; a control group was given an alternate task unrelated to inhibitory control. Researchers adjusted the difficulty to individual skills, allowing participants to find their own level of training.

Activity changes in the brain were tracked using fMRI (Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging). The research team found that activity in two areas of the brain known to be associated with inhibitory control was lowered during the exercise but raised just before the inhibition control period. During inhibition control, further changes were shown, but these changes were relatively small.

According to the research team, the training linked the performance improvements to specific cues predicting that inhibitory control was going to be needed-basically, for that specific task under those specific conditions, the brain learned how to better predict when to stop the task. This implies the improvement may not necessarily be generalized. For example, an Olympic sprinter who used this style of training to get a better jump off of the blocks may not see any improvement unless the same cues are available in the training and the Olympic event.

This work doesn't suggest that brain-training programs aren't useful, but it does suggest that transfer into real-world improvements may not always be straightforward.
About the Author
You May Also Like
SEP 22, 2019
Health & Medicine
SEP 22, 2019
Wi-Fi Poses A Growing Health Risk
Groups linked to the telecommunications industry claim there are no possible health impacts related to Wi-Fi use. Several recent studies signal that the op...
SEP 22, 2019
Health & Medicine
SEP 22, 2019
How Much Caffeine Can Trigger a Migraine?
Do you suffer from migraines? Your caffeine intake may be a trigger. Researchers from the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC), Brigham and Women&r...
SEP 22, 2019
Health & Medicine
SEP 22, 2019
New drug to treat opioid use disorder during pregnancy is favorable for mom, and baby
On average, a baby is born in withdrawal from opioids every 15 minutes in the U.S., according to recent research. Opioid use during pregnancy can result in...
SEP 22, 2019
Genetics & Genomics
SEP 22, 2019
The Genetic Reasons Why Some People Need Less Sleep
Typically, people need between seven and eight hours of sleep per night to function properly. In  fact, reducing sleep by just one or two hours for a ...
SEP 22, 2019
Neuroscience
SEP 22, 2019
Can CRISPR Technology Treat Alzheimer's?
New drugs that seek to treat Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have a 99.6% failure rate. This makes it the only disease in the top 10 causes of death that ca...
SEP 22, 2019
Neuroscience
SEP 22, 2019
Why do we forget our dreams? The brain may hinder memories during dream sleep
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is a phase when dreams are created. Traditionally, REM has been thought of as a sleep stage important for memory consolidation, or the long-term storage of memo...
Loading Comments...